Barry L. Beyerstein of the Brain Behavior
Laboratory at Simon Fraser University in Vancouver explains.
Whenever I venture out of the Ivory Tower to deliver public lectures
about the brain, by far the most likely question I can expect as the talk
winds up is, "Do we really only use 10 percent of our brains?" The look of
disappointment that usually follows when I say it isn't so strongly
suggests that the 10-percent myth is one of those hopeful shibboleths that
refuses to die simply because it would be so darn nice if it were true.
I'm sure none of us would turn down a mighty hike in brainpower if it were
attainable, and a seemingly never-ending stream of crackpot schemes and
devices continues to be advanced by hucksters who trade on the myth.
Always on the lookout for a "feel-good" story, the media have also played
their part in keeping the myth alive. A study of self-improvement products
by a panel of the prestigious National Research Council, Enhancing
Human Performance, surveyed an assortment of the less far-fetched
offerings of the "brain booster" genre and came to the conclusion that
(alas!) there is no reliable substitute for practice and hard work when it
comes to getting ahead in life. This unwelcome news has done little,
however, to dissuade millions who are comforted by the prospect that the
shortcut to their unfulfilled dreams lies in the fact that they just
haven't quite found the secret to tap this vast, allegedly unused cerebral
reservoir.
Why would a neuroscientist immediately doubt that 90 percent of the
average brain lies perpetually fallow? First of all, it is obvious that
the brain, like all our other organs, has been shaped by natural
selection. Brain tissue is metabolically expensive both to grow and to
run, and it strains credulity to think that evolution would have permitted
squandering of resources on a scale necessary to build and maintain such a
massively underutilized organ. Moreover, doubts are fueled by ample
evidence from clinical neurology. Losing far less than 90 percent of the
brain to accident or disease has catastrophic consequences. What is more,
observing the effects of head injury reveals that there does not seem to
be any area of the brain that can be destroyed by strokes, head trauma, or
other manner, without leaving the patient with some kind of functional
deficit. Likewise, electrical stimulation of points in the brain during
neurosurgery has failed so far to uncover any dormant areas where no
percept, emotion or movement is elicited by applying these tiny currents
(this can be done with conscious patients under local anesthetic because
the brain itself has no pain receptors).
The past hundred years has seen the advent of increasingly
sophisticated technologies for listening in on the functional traffic of
the brain. The goal of behavioral neuroscience has been to record
electrical, chemical and magnetic changes in brain activity and to
correlate them with specific mental and behavioral phenomena. With the aid
of instruments such as EEGs, magnetoencephalographs, PET scanners and
functional MRI machines, researchers have succeeded in localizing a vast
number of psychological functions to specific centers and systems in the
brain. With nonhuman animals, and occasionally with human patients
undergoing neurological treatment, recording probes can even be inserted
into the brain itself. Despite this detailed reconnaissance, no quiet
areas awaiting new assignments have emerged.
All told, the foregoing suggests that there is no cerebral spare tire
waiting to be mounted in service of one's grade point average, job
advancement, or the pursuit of a cure for cancer or the Great American
Novel. So, if the 10-percent myth is that implausible, how did it arise?
My attempts to track down the origins of the 10-percent myth have not
discovered any smoking guns, but some tantalizing clues have emerged (more
are recounted in the references below). One stream leads back to the
pioneering American psychologist, William James, in the late 19th and
early 20th centuries. In addition to his voluminous scholarly work, James
was a prodigious author of popular articles offering advice to the general
public. In these exhortatory works James was fond of stating that the
average person rarely achieves but a small portion of his or her
potential. I was never able to find an exact percentage mentioned, and
James always talked in terms of one's undeveloped potential, apparently
never relating this to a specific amount of gray matter engaged. A
generation of "positive thinking" gurus that followed were not so careful,
however, and gradually "10 percent of our capacity" morphed into "10
percent of our brain." Undoubtedly, the biggest boost for the self-help
entrepreneurs came when the famous adventurer and journalist Lowell Thomas
attributed the 10-percent-of-the-brain claim to William James. Thomas did
so in the preface he wrote, in 1936, to one of the best-selling self-help
books of all time, Dale Carnegie's How to Win Friends and Influence
People. The myth has never lost its steam since.
Other sources for the ubiquity of the 10-percent myth probably come
from popular authors' misconstrual of scientific papers by early brain
researchers. For example, in calling (for technical reasons) a huge
percentage of the cerebral hemispheres the "silent cortex," early
investigators may have left the mistaken impression that what is now
referred to as the "association cortex" had no function. That was far from
the researchers' intention, but that is what seems to have filtered
through to the public. Likewise, early researchers' appropriately modest
admissions that they didn't know what 90 percent of the brain was doing
probably fostered the widespread misconception that the leftovers did
nothing.
In my quest for the seminal utterance of the 10-percent myth, I
frequently came across the claim that Albert Einstein had once explained
his own brilliance by reference to the myth--Einstein's enormous prestige,
of course, making it unassailable thenceforth. A careful search by the
helpful people at the Albert Einstein archives, however, was unable to
provide me with any record of such a statement on his part. So it remains
probably just another of those instances where promoters with a point or a
buck to make have misappropriated the clout of Einstein's name to further
their own endeavors.
The 10-percent myth has undoubtedly motivated many people to strive for
greater creativity and productivity in their lives--hardly a bad thing.
The comfort, encouragement and hope that it has engendered helps explain
its longevity. But, like so many uplifting myths that are too good to be
true, the truth of the matter seems to be its least important aspect.
Answer posted on March 08, 2004